Enhancing the Conceptualization and Measurement of Implicit and Explicit Affective Evaluations: A Case Study in Disordered Eating

نویسندگان

  • Halley E. Woodward
  • C. Daryl Cameron
  • Teresa A. Treat
چکیده

Affective evaluations (i.e., evaluations of affectively evocative stimuli) play important roles in many behaviors, including clinically relevant behaviors like disordered eating. Understanding automatic and controlled affective evaluations can enhance prediction and treatment of more deliberate versus more impulsive clinical behaviors, respectively. However, methodological and theoretical shortcomings cloud the implicit affective evaluation literature, particularly as applied in clinical psychology. This article aims to improve the clinical science of implicit and explicit affective evaluation by capitalizing on theoretical and methodological advancements drawn from implicit social cognition.We recommend three key enhancements to the affective evaluation literature: improve the structural comparability between indirect and direct measures, assess evaluations on both valence and arousal dimensions of affect, and carefully characterize and select stimuli. Additional improvements to analytic approaches (e.g., mixed effects modeling and process dissociation) are also recommended. Such efforts will advance our theoretical understanding of the relative contributions of automatic and controlled processes to clinically relevant affective evaluation, thereby informing assessment and improving intervention. Applying advanced methodologies of implicit cognition to clinical phenomenawill also reiterate and reinforce the use of these advances in social psychology. Enhancing the Conceptualization and Measurement of Clinically Relevant Implicit and Explicit Affective Evaluations: A Case Study in Disordered Eating Most of us have eaten mindlessly – when an engrossing movie grabs our attention, we may be surprised to find we have emptied the popcorn bucket accidentally. Most of us have also eaten mindfully – when savoring a glass of wine, we might consciously appreciate its nuances and f lavors. These are but two examples of how affective evaluations of food can determine eating behavior. Indeed, research finds that we are more likely to approach (Chen & Bargh, 1999) and consume (e.g., Drewnowski, Henderson, Levine, & Hann, 1999) foods that we evaluate positively. In the current review, we examine how methodological insights from implicit social cognition can inform the study of affective evaluations of food in clinical populations, and in turn, how applications in clinical psychology can inform theory andmeasurement in social psychology. The processes underlying affective evaluations (i.e., evaluations of affectively evocative stimuli) fall along a spectrum from purely automatic to purely controlled, and a number of dual process models have been proposed to explain the interactive inf luences of these processes (e.g., Jacoby, 1991; Strack & Deutsch, 2004). Automatic affective evaluations play a role in many clinical phenomena beyond the domain of eating behavior, such as addictive disorders (Wiers & de Jong, 2006), post-traumatic stress disorder (Lindgren, Kaysen, Werntz, Gasser, & Teachman, 2013), and others ( for a review see Roefs et al., 2011). We can be in a better © 2016 John Wiley & Sons Ltd Enhancing Measurement of Affective Evaluation 253 position to understand, predict, and treat clinical disorders when we understand the automatic and controlled affective processes underlying disordered behaviors (e.g., Payne, Jacoby, & Lambert, 2005). Conversely, basic research in social cognition can benefit from rigorous clinical investigations of automatic and controlled affective processes, as clinical phenomena often differ from normative social behavior in degree, not kind. For example, the types of behaviors examined (and the methods by which they are examined) differ minimally between social psychologists interested in self-regulation (e.g., Hofmann, Rauch, & Gawronski, 2007) and clinical psychologists interested in understanding the extremes of eating and weight-related behavior (e.g., Roefs & Jansen, 2002). Thus, while this is a paper about ways to improve clinical research in the domain of disordered eating behavior, we hope that the considerations outlined here will prove useful to social psychologists by reiterating cutting edge advances in implicit measurement through their application to clinically relevant phenomena. The field of social cognition has examined extensively the circumstances under which implicit and explicit evaluations or attitudes converge and diverge. A number of models explain the interplay of deliberative and automatic processes in implicit and explicit evaluation, including theMODEmodel (Fazio, 1990), the associative-propositional evaluationmodel (Gawronski & Bodenhausen, 2006), and the meta-cognitive model (Petty, Briñol, & DeMarree, 2007). Implicit and explicit evaluations should diverge when there are social pressures demanding an explicit response that might differ from implicit reactions (e.g., Dovidio, Kawakami, Johnson, Johnson, & Howard, 1997); when individuals fear social judgment for their responses (e.g., Loersch, McCaslin, & Petty, 2011); and when situational (e.g., alcohol consumption, working memory load, and emotion suppression) or dispositional (e.g., working memory capacity) moderators render reliance on controlled processes more difficult (e.g., Friese & Hofmann, 2009; Friese, Hofmann, & Schmitt, 2008; Friese, Hofmann, & Wänke, 2008; Hofmann & Friese, 2008; Hofmann, Friese, & Roefs, 2009; Hofmann, Friese, & Strack, 2009; Hofmann, Gschwendner, Friese, Wiers, & Schmitt, 2008; Hofmann et al., 2007). However, uncontrolled method variance might be contributing to this theoretically expected implicit-explicit divergence in the disordered eating literature. If indirect and direct measures differ substantially in terms of their administration format, then the observed divergences may ref lect methodological differences, rather than differences in the constructs of implicit and explicit evaluations (Payne, Burkley, & Stokes, 2008). In other words, methodological and theoretical shortcomings limit the existing applications of implicit measurement to the study of clinically relevant affective evaluations. Fortunately, approaches commonly used in the social cognition literature can address these limitations. We brief ly critique the literature on disordered eating as a test case for illustratingmore general methodological and theoretical concerns about implicit measurement in clinical psychology.We then pull from the social cognition literature to highlight three critical theoretical and methodological issues in the disordered eating literature: measurement concerns, incomplete examination of affective dimensions, and poorly characterized and selected stimuli. How readily can we compare the results of indirect and direct measures? Which dimension(s) of affect matter: valence, arousal, or both? And could our stimulus selection processes be clouding the conclusions we draw? Addressing these crucial issues will advance our knowledge of the processes underlying affective evaluations and enhance our prediction and understanding of both clinical and social phenomena. Our recommendations provide more conservative tests of theories specifying the conditions under which divergence between implicit and explicit processes would emerge. The measurement concerns we will highlight below involve two measurement strategies – indirect and direct – that help us to understand processes that fall along a continuum from fully automatic to fully controlled. Indirect measures (i.e., tasks on which the construct of interest is inferred from © 2016 John Wiley & Sons Ltd Social and Personality Psychology Compass (2016): 252–264, 10.1111/spc3.12245 254 Enhancing Measurement of Affective Evaluation participant behavior, such as the implicit association test [IAT; Greenwald, McGhee, & Schwartz, 1998]; De Houwer, 2006) are thought to assess relatively more automatic processes, and direct measures (i.e., tasks that require participants to report, assess, or rate the construct of interest; De Houwer, 2006) are thought to assess relatively more controlled processes. Here, we use implicit and explicit as synonyms for automatic and controlled, respectively (De Houwer, 2006). Wholly automatic evaluations lack efficiency; awareness (of the stimulus, one’s evaluation thereof, or the cause of one’s evaluation); intention (i.e., the intent to initiate the evaluation); and control (i.e., the ability to cease or override evaluation once started; Bargh, 1994). Controlled evaluations, in contrast, may require cognitive resources for their implementation, may operate consciously and/or intentionally, and/or can be volitionally controlled (Bargh, 1994; Moors & De Houwer, 2006). Notably, processes may demonstrate one or more feature of automaticity or control and are rarely characterized by all four features. Because affective evaluation processes fall along a spectrum from fully automatic to fully controlled, we use the modifier “relatively more” when describing presumed automatic or controlled affective evaluations. Much of the disordered eating literature has assumed that indirect measures assess only automatic processes, and direct measures assess only controlled processes. However, this process purity assumption is problematic; performance on almost any task involves both relatively more automatic and relatively more controlled processes (e.g., Jacoby, 1991; Sherman, 2008). Moreover, which process predominates is inf luenced by the extent to which respondents have the opportunity and inclination to resist their automatic response (Fazio, 1990). Similarly, eating behavior can range from extremely spontaneous and impulsive to highly deliberate and restrictive. The American Psychiatric Association recognizes several eating disorder diagnoses, including anorexia nervosa, bulimia nervosa, binge eating disorder, and other specified eating disorder (i.e., clinically significant cases that fail to meet formal diagnostic criteria; American Psychiatric Association, 2013). The disordered eating literature also considers unhealthy eating patterns that fail to meet diagnostic criteria but nevertheless impact sufferers’ well-being (e.g., dietary restraint, in which people both chronically diet in an attempt to suppress their weight, and overeat in ways that are similar to binge eating; Herman & Polivy, 1980). Thus, we will examine food-related affective evaluations along a spectrum of eating pathology that includes non-clinical samples, from disinhibited eating behaviors (e.g., those with binge eating disorder and bulimia nervosa as well as restrained eaters and those who eat in response to environmental or emotional cues; Masheb & Grilo, 2006; van Strien, Frijters, Bergers, & Defares, 1986) to successfully restrictive eating behaviors (e.g., those with anorexia nervosa, those who successfully diet). The continuum of automatic and controlled affective evaluations can inform our understanding of the disordered eating spectrum; normative eating patterns; and related, socially relevant constructs such as self-regulation. However, the inferences we can draw from this literature are clouded by uncontrolled method variance resulting from procedural discrepancies in measurement strategies, overlooked affective dimensions, and messily characterized stimuli. These theoretical and methodological shortcomings matter. Identifying the roles of automatic and controlled affective evaluations can improve prediction of spontaneous and deliberative eating behaviors (Cameron, Brown-Iannuzzi, & Payne, 2012; Hofmann, Gawronski, Gschwendner, Le, & Schmitt, 2005). For normative eating, spontaneous eating behavior might include consuming potato chips in front of the television, whereas deliberative eating behavior might include declining a slice of birthday cake in favor of the salad one packed for lunch. For disordered eating, disinhibited eating behaviors often involve losing control over consumption during a binge eating episode, whereas restrictive eating behaviors include deliberately and successfully restricting food intake even to the point of starvation. © 2016 John Wiley & Sons Ltd Social and Personality Psychology Compass (2016): 252–264, 10.1111/spc3.12245 Enhancing Measurement of Affective Evaluation 255 Importantly, addressing methodological problems can inf luence what kinds of interventions are used (e.g., Rothman, Sheeran, &Wood, 2009), as therapeutic approaches targeting automatic behaviors differ from those targeting controlled behaviors (Friese, Hofmann, & Wiers, 2011; Moldovan & David, 2012). For instance, attention-based interventions may reduce impulsive eating (e.g., Kemps, Tiggemann, Orr, & Grear, 2014) through retraining of automatic processes, whereas mindfulness interventions may reduce impulsive eating (e.g., Katterman, Kleinman, Hood, Nackers, & Corsica, 2014) by enhancing executive control. Additionally, what we infer about affective evaluations on the basis of coarsely selected or limited stimulus sets may overlook important nutritional characteristics such as fat content, sugar content, or level of processing, which potentially inf luence affective evaluation. Finally, social psychologists may benefit from such advances in clinical science, as the relevance of underlying processes or nutritional characteristics to disordered eating behavior likely extends to normative variants of eating behavior. There is some support for the dual-process conceptualization of food-related affective evaluations. Roefs and colleagues (2011) reviewed the literature on implicit associations relevant to disordered eating. Generally, they identified more negative evaluations of foods (with occasional preferences for low-calorie foods) among people who successfully restrict their eating, including those with anorexia nervosa and mixed evaluations among disinhibited eaters. Healthy participants’ implicit food-related affective evaluations only sometimes differed from those of obese participants, chronic failed dieters, and those who eat in response to environmental cues. This work suggests that indirectly and directly measured affective evaluations sometimes converge to predict self-reported consumption (Conner, Perugini, O’Gorman, Ayres, & Prestwich, 2007, exp. 2). Still, indirect and direct measures diverge more frequently than they converge (see Roefs et al., 2011). When the findings diverge, typically the direct measure better predicts the outcome of interest, which could suggest that controlled processes may be especially important (e.g., Spruyt, Hermans, De Houwer, Vandekerckhove, & Eelen, 2007, exp. 1), at least in contexts that afford opportunities and motivation to rely on controlled processes (Fazio, 1990). An additional explanation may be at work: If self-reported affective evaluations predict self-reported behavior, then this common feature of measurement (i.e., self-report) may produce or increase a relationship. And if you compare a Likert scale index with an IAT index, then discrepancies between these implicit and explicit measures may ref lect the many methodological differences between these measurement strategies (e.g., response scale and administration method), as well as the differing contributions of automatic and controlled affective evaluations of foods. We will return to this issue shortly. Improving the Study of Affective Evaluation in the Disordered Eating Literature Three key modifications would significantly advance the field of clinically relevant implicit evaluations: improving the structural fit and comparability between indirect and direct measures, examining multiple dimensions of affect, and selecting well-characterized multidimensional stimuli. Here, we focus on the eating-related literature, but we anticipate that the following suggestions would improve the study of affective evaluations across many content areas within clinical and social psychology. Increasing Structural Fit Between Indirect and Direct Measures Very often in the clinical literature, direct self-report measures relying on Likert scale ratings are juxtaposed with indirect reaction-time tasks. For example, in one of the seminal papers addressing eatingand weight-related affective evaluations, Roefs and Jansen (2002) contrasted explicit 9-point palatability ratings with an IAT. Other work pairs an IAT with explicit bipolar rating © 2016 John Wiley & Sons Ltd Social and Personality Psychology Compass (2016): 252–264, 10.1111/spc3.12245 256 Enhancing Measurement of Affective Evaluation scales (e.g., Richetin, Perugini, Prestwich, &O’Gorman, 2007) or single target IATs with questionnaires assessing eating behavior (e.g., Bongers, Jansen, Houben, & Roefs, 2013). This task dissociation approach compares an indirect and a direct measure and assumes that the former indexes implicit processes while the latter indexes explicit processes. Sometimes this task dissociation approach is combined with examinations of moderators that should only inf luence automatic or controlled processes, such as when self-regulatory resources are manipulated to see if automatic candy attitudes better predict candy consumption when regulatory resources are low (as predicted by the MODE model; Hofmann, Rauch & Gawronski, 2007; Fazio, 1990). Examining such moderators provides greater nuance in the investigation of dual process models of affective evaluations of foods but does not address the discrepancies between indirect and direct measurement strategies that can contribute to implicit-explicit dissociations. Weak associations between indirect and direct measures are often interpreted as evidence that implicit and explicit processes diverge strongly. But poor structural fit can also contribute to such divergence (e.g., Payne et al., 2005; Payne, Jacoby, & Lambert, 2005). For example, an unimpressive relationship between a food IAT and an explicit Likert scale rating of food could result from true differences between implicit and explicit affective evaluations of food, or from uncontrolledmethod variance associated with different response scales, administration methods, stimuli to be evaluated, et cetera. In contrast, we could draw stronger inferences about the relevance of a dual process model to food-related affective evaluations if our indirect and direct affective evaluation tasks were highly comparable. If the indirect and direct measures only differ on the process dimension of interest – e.g., intentionality – but not in terms of measurement particularities, then we can make stronger and more valid claims about dual processes of affective evaluation. One example of more comparable indirect and direct measurement strategies is provided by the affect misattribution procedure (AMP). The AMP draws upon well-established principles of affect misattribution to index implicit attitudes (Payne et al., 2005). The AMP successfully converges with explicit attitudes and predicts behavioral outcomes (Cameron, Brown-Iannuzzi, & Payne, 2012), and it has been used to address clinical questions about smoking (Payne, McClernon, & Dobbins, 2007) and alcohol consumption (Payne, Govorun, & Arbuckle, 2008). We focus on the AMP as an example of how to achieve structural correspondence for three reasons. First, published work has shown how to achieve such structural fit with the AMP (Payne et al., 2008), providing a good example for future work. Second, the AMP focuses specifically on affective evaluations, making it pertinent to clinically relevant affective evaluations. Third, the AMP is a modern, highly versatile indirect measure with convergent and predictive validity comparable to other measures such as the IAT (Cameron et al., 2012). Although it was originally developed to assess implicit affective evaluations, the AMP can assess implicit and explicit affective evaluations using two structurally similar conditions that differ only in their instructions (Payne et al., 2005; Payne et al., 2008; Payne & Lundberg, 2014). In both indirect and direct versions of the task, participants see a brief ly presented photo followed by a brief ly presented and masked Chinese character, which serves as a neutral stimulus. On the indirect AMP, participants are instructed to rate the valence (pleasant/unpleasant) of the Chinese character and to avoid allowing the photo to inf luence their rating. A misattribution mechanism underlies the indirect AMP, such that affect elicited automatically by the photo is mistakenly applied to evaluations of the Chinese character (e.g., Gawronski & Ye, 2013). Automatic (i.e., unintentional and unconscious) affective evaluations of the photos can be inferred from indirect AMP performancewhenmisattributions occur despite instructions to prevent effects of the photos (Payne, Jacoby, & Lambert, 2005; Payne & Bishara, 2009). The direct AMP is nearly identical to the indirect AMP except for one feature: intentionality. In the direct AMP, participants are instructed to directly evaluate the prime photographs and to ignore the inf luence of the Chinese characters. Thus, the two AMPs differ only in whether © 2016 John Wiley & Sons Ltd Social and Personality Psychology Compass (2016): 252–264, 10.1111/spc3.12245 Enhancing Measurement of Affective Evaluation 257 evaluation of prime images is intentional (direct) or unintentional (indirect; see Woodward & Treat, 2015 for an illustration of improved structural fit using the AMP). By controlling all other potential sources of method variance, inferences about dual processes from dissociations between the two measures are more valid and justified. In sum, using such comparable indirect and direct measures allows for more rigorous inferences about dual processes of affective evaluations, because methodological artifacts are ruled out as explanatory possibilities. To the degree that past work on dual processes in clinical contexts uses indirect and direct measures with low structural fit, it is difficult to establish why previously observed dissociations actually occur. The structural constraints of many other widely used implicit measures, such as the IAT, may not lend themselves as readily to structural fit enhancements. However, new measures may be developed by reducing differences between indirect and direct assessments that are unrelated to theoretical distinctions between implicit and explicit evaluations. For instance, Likert scale ratings of foods could be administered with andwithout time pressure, to match indirect and direct measures on all but the automaticity feature of resource efficiency (e.g., Ranganath & Nosek, 2008). Attending to structural fit helps to minimize uncontrolled method variance, which in turn may increase correlations between indirect and direct measures. Such increased co rrelations are simply a byproduct of increased measurement comparability, and appropriately restrict explanatory possibilities. Payne and colleagues (2008, exp. 3) demonstrated this point by examining correlations among indirect and direct AMP tasks assessing evaluations of alcohol and race. They found significant correlations between indirect and direct measures of the same construct (race or alcohol), but no significant correlations across constructs, even when the methods were identical (e.g., indirect race and indirect alcohol evaluations). When our indirect and direct assessments are more structurally similar, discrepancies between the results of these measures are more likely to ref lect differences at the level of affective processes. Improved structural fit thus provides a more conservative test of dual process models, because we can specify which feature(s) of automaticity differ between our implicit and explicit evaluations, and we can be sure that their differences are not unduly inf lated by method variance unrelated to our theoretical constructs. Improving structural fit is a critical step forward for making more valid inferences about dual processes. However, even structurally similar indirect and direct assessments of affective evaluations do not provide pure estimates of automatic and controlled processes (e.g., De Houwer, 2006; Jacoby, 1991; Payne, 2008). Both implicit attitudes and executive control could drive performance on indirect measures such as the indirect AMP or the IAT; the same could be said for performance on direct measures such as a direct AMP or self-report scale. Improving structural fit would be a major step forward, one that to date has not been fully appreciated within social or clinical psychology. Estimation of formal process-dissociation models (Jacoby, 1991; Payne, 2008; Payne & Cameron, 2014) using multinomial modeling (Batchelder & Riefer, 1999; Conrey, Sherman, Gawronski, Hugenberg, & Groom, 2005; Payne & Bishara, 2009; Riefer & Batchelder, 1988) would further strengthen our inferences by providing quantitative estimates of the interactive contributions of automatic and controlled processes to performance on a single task. Examination of Valence and Arousal Dimensions of Affect Our knowledge of affective evaluations is obscured further by the nearly exclusive attention paid to the positive–negative valence dimension of affect. Most existing models of affect also converge on an independent arousal dimension (i.e., degree of activation or motivational intensity; e.g., Lang, 1995; also Russell, 2003), yet the majority of research on affective evaluations in social and clinical psychology has focused on the former at the expense of the latter. Each © 2016 John Wiley & Sons Ltd Social and Personality Psychology Compass (2016): 252–264, 10.1111/spc3.12245 258 Enhancing Measurement of Affective Evaluation dimension of affect can contribute to our understanding of clinically relevant affective evaluations. For example, food craving occurs when the affective evaluation of that food includes positive valence and some degree of arousal (e.g., Craeynest, Crombez, Koster, Haerens, & De Bourdeaudhuij, 2008). This conceptualization of food craving echoes that of another predominant theory of craving and food incentive. The theory of food reward proposed by Berridge (1996) holds that two independent neural processes determine interest in eating: liking and wanting. Liking results from the processing of hedonic pleasantness (e.g., sweet tastes), while wanting is more akin to appetite or incentive salience, in that wanted foods become attention-grabbing or craved (though not necessarily consciously; Robinson & Berridge, 2008; Berridge, Ho, Richard, & DiFeliceantonio, 2010). Both processes can be automatic in the sense that they can occur without conscious awareness (Berridge, 1996). Wiers and colleagues (2002) illustrated the importance of assessing the arousal dimension in a test of liking and wanting in alcohol addiction. Arousal evaluations measured with an arousal IAT significantly differed between heavy and light drinkers, particularly among men, while all participants showed an indirect negative evaluation of alcohol on the valence IAT. In other words, wanting (akin to arousal) – but not liking (akin to valence) – distinguished light and heavy drinkers. To the extent that craving for food resembles craving for other addictive substances (e.g., Gearhardt et al., 2011), we can learn more about food-related affective evaluations by assessing both arousal and valence dimensions (see Wiers & de Jong, 2006 for a review that unpacks the importance of both valence and arousal to the study of addictive behavior). The food-related implicit affective evaluation literature also supports further investigation of the arousal dimension of affect. Within two studies of severely obese, overweight, and normalweight youths, Craeynest and colleagues (2008) explored the effects of stimulus arousal on implicit affective evaluations. Both studies found that all youths, regardless of weight, associated arousal more with high-fat than with lean food images, and associated high-fat foods with both positive and negative high arousal (Craeynest et al., 2008, exp. 1 and 2). The potential importance of stimulus arousal, in addition to valence, is echoed in an affective priming paradigm study of undergraduate women, which found that implicit affective evaluations of food images reliably differed between BMI groups only on trials with high-arousal target words (Czyzewska &Graham, 2008). Pending replication, these findings demonstrate the utility of examining both the arousal and the valence dimensions of clinically relevant affective evaluations. Enhanced Selection of Stimuli Word stimuli are used commonly throughout the food-related affective evaluation literature, though images would likely enhance the ecological validity and generalizability of this research (e.g., Veenstra & de Jong, 2010), and working with actual foods would move research even further in the direction of ecological validity. Individuals rarely interact with foods in writing; menus and food magazines are littered with photos. Imagine seeing an enticing slice of cake versus reading the name of its recipe. The visual cue is more compelling than the verbal description. Images more effectively activate automatic affective responses than words because images tap into the semantic systems where affect is processed more directly than words (e.g., Bradley & Lang, 2007; De Houwer & Hermans, 1994). Word stimuli may not adequately capture the processes that inform real-world judgments about what to eat, given how frequently these decisions are informed by visual cues. Additionally, stimuli are rarely well characterized within this literature: many potential stimulus aspects are unspecified or uncontrolled for both word and image stimuli. Recently, efforts have been made to develop taxonomies of foods (e.g., Foroni, Pergola, Argiris, Rumiati, © 2016 John Wiley & Sons Ltd Social and Personality Psychology Compass (2016): 252–264, 10.1111/spc3.12245 Enhancing Measurement of Affective Evaluation 259 2013), but often distinctions among foods are imprecise. Food stimuli may be classified as highversus low-calorie (e.g., Spring & Bulik, 2014) or as healthy versus unhealthy (e.g., Perugini, O’Gorman & Prestwich, 2007), leaving fat, fiber, sugar, and sometimes caloric content unspecified. Not all high-calorie or high-fat foods are created equal, however. For example, restrained and unrestrained eaters differ in their responses to cookies but not to pizza, both of which are presumed to be high in both fat and calories but which differ in sugar content (Fedoroff, Polivy, & Herman, 2003). This suggests that properties beyond fatand calorie-content or overall unhealthiness may inf luence affective evaluations. Instead, stimuli should vary along multiple reasonably independent relevant dimensions, such as degree of processing, fat content, and sugar content (e.g., Gearhardt, Treat, Hollingworth, & Corbin, 2012; Rizk & Treat, 2013;Woodward & Treat, 2015), or perhaps normative valence and arousal ratings (Bradley & Lang, 2007). One can simultaneously examine participant-specific and stimulus-specific predictors of affective evaluations, as well as their interactions, by incorporating nutritional or normative affective information about carefully selected stimuli into mixed-effects analytic procedures. For example, careful selection of stimuli that vary largely independently along fat and sugar dimensions would allow us to use mixed-effects analyses to determine whether sugar or fat content more strongly inf luences affective evaluations across participants (see Fedoroff et al., 2003; Gearhardt, Rizk, & Treat, 2014). We also could assess whether these nutrient-specific effects vary as a function of individual differences (i.e., whether participant-specific and stimulus-specific predictors interact to predict affective evaluations). For example, perhaps the effect of foods’ fat content on affective evaluations is stronger for those who restrict their eating, relative to those who do not. Perhaps the effect of binge eating on affective evaluations is specific to high-sugar foods. Such food-specific and participant-specific hypotheses can be tested when we rely on more comprehensive and well-controlled stimulus selection procedures and analyze data using mixed-effects methods. A Template for Affective Evaluation Research A researcher interested in conducting a well-controlled study of implicit and explicit affective evaluations of food stimuli would want to take the following steps: First, she ought to consider the paradigmatic features of indirect and direct measures, by selecting or constructing measures that are as structurally similar to one another as possible. Doing so will reduce uncontrolled method variance and strengthen our inferences about the primary theoretical constructs of interest. After maximizing structural fit, process dissociation or multinomial modeling could be employed to decompose the inf luences of underlying processes of automatic and controlled evaluation on task performance, to the extent that such models are available. In the current paper, we have focused on structural fit as an initial step at controlling method variance; process dissociation is a subsequent step that we strongly endorse to enhance the construct validity of our indices when feasible ( for review, see Payne & Cameron, 2014). Incorporating these advances in implicit measurement will allow a more valid and precise evaluation of dual process theories about clinically relevant affect evaluations. Critically, although much prior work makes dual process claims about affective evaluations in clinical contexts, this work may not license strong inferences to the extent that structural fit between indirect and direct measures is poor. Moreover, illustrating the theoretical advantages of these measurement advances in clinical contexts may reinforce and reiterate the use of these advances in social psychology, where they are still not readily applied. Second, the researcher ought to assess both pleasant-unpleasant valence evaluations and activating-unactivating arousal evaluations. Affect involves valence and arousal, and an © 2016 John Wiley & Sons Ltd Social and Personality Psychology Compass (2016): 252–264, 10.1111/spc3.12245 260 Enhancing Measurement of Affective Evaluation appropriate treatment of implicit affective evaluations requires assessing both. This claim applies both to clinical psychology and to social psychology, where arousal may play important roles in clinical behaviors (e.g., Berridge, 1996) and normative social processes (Cheng, Ottati, & Price, 2013). Assessing both primary affective dimensions will provide a more nuanced characterization of the construct of interest. For instance, one could imagine that pleasant and activating evaluations could signify craving for a particular food, while unpleasant and activating evaluations could ref lect disgust or learned taste aversion. Our researcher would not be able to address these questions without assessing both affective dimensions. Finally, she would benefit from employing a large number of food images with known nutritional and affective properties so that nutrient-specific questions could be asked. Our researcher would be well powered to detect nutrient-specific, as well as individual-specific, effects if she were to utilize a mixed-effects modeling approach to analyze data from a sufficiently large sample. Indeed, she would even be well positioned to determine whether individual differences, such as different forms of eating pathology, inf luence affective evaluations of only food with certain properties. Our researcher’s efforts to minimize measurement discrepancies between indirect and direct measures, to assess the two primary affective dimensions, to select a wide array of image stimuli with known properties, and to utilize cutting edge analytic models would result in a methodologically rigorous investigation of dual process models of affective evaluation from which she could draw stronger inferences.

برای دانلود رایگان متن کامل این مقاله و بیش از 32 میلیون مقاله دیگر ابتدا ثبت نام کنید

ثبت نام

اگر عضو سایت هستید لطفا وارد حساب کاربری خود شوید

منابع مشابه

Unhealthy how? Implicit and explicit affective evaluations of different types of unhealthy foods.

The present study examines the utility of distinguishing nutritional characteristics in implicit and explicit affective evaluations of foods, both nomothetically and idiographically. To this end, we employ indirect and direct versions of the affect misattribution procedure (AMP; Payne, Cheng, Govorun, & Stewart, 2005) to assess affective evaluations of foods that vary along dimensions of added ...

متن کامل

Valence and arousal-based affective evaluations of foods.

We investigated the nutrient-specific and individual-specific validity of dual-process models of valenced and arousal-based affective evaluations of foods across the disordered eating spectrum. 283 undergraduate women provided implicit and explicit valence and arousal-based evaluations of 120 food photos with known nutritional information on structurally similar indirect and direct affect misat...

متن کامل

Measuring the Effectiveness of Explicit and Implicit Instruction through Explicit and Implicit Measures

Many studies have examined the effect of different approaches to teaching grammar including explicit and implicit instruction. However, research in this area is limited in a number of respects. One such limitation pertains to the issue of construct validity of the measures, i.e. the knowledge developed through implicit instruction has been measured through instruments which favor th...

متن کامل

Proposing a Model of Co-Creative Participation in Tourism Market

Objective There is a growing interest in the customers’ innovation in the realm of tourism studies. In the new global business ecosystem where individuals, organizations, governments and economy work together as an integrated network, we need a new innovation model. The model should be set at a level in which internal, external, cooperative and co-creative ideas can converge to create organiza...

متن کامل

Role of emotion regulation difficulties as a mediator of the relationship alexithymia and disturbance eating among students

The prevalence of disordered eating behaviors has been reported to increase in recent decades; on the other hand one situation that can impede the regulation of emotions is alexithymia. Therefore, the search for specific psychological variables that may contribute to the etiology of this disorder is of great importance. The current study examined the mediating role of the emotion regulation dif...

متن کامل

ذخیره در منابع من


  با ذخیره ی این منبع در منابع من، دسترسی به آن را برای استفاده های بعدی آسان تر کنید

عنوان ژورنال:

دوره   شماره 

صفحات  -

تاریخ انتشار 2016